Science Gives Clues To World Cup Success

Science gives clues to World Cup success


Updated Mon May 10, 2010 3:57pm AEST
http://www.abc.net.au/news/stories/2010/05/10/2895277.htm

Penalty kicks, such as Francesco Totti's in the 2006 World Cup, can make or break a team (Kai Pfaffenbach/Reuters)
Few moments in soccer are as extraordinary as the penalty, the moment when a dream can crumble or glory is made - and a player is either cursed as a choker or enters the pantheon of legends.
In the nearly 119 years since the very first penalty kick, in a match between Wolverhampton Wanderers and Accrington Stanley, the 11-metre spot determines more and more tournaments, including the 2006 World Cup final.
As the importance of the penalty has grown, so has research. Scientists see it as a duel between shooter and goalkeeper where biomechanics and psychology can give either side a critical edge.
A mathematical study of penalties at Liverpool's John Moores University puts the death nail into the "blast it and hope" approach.

The perfect penalty, it found, is a ball that is struck high, targeted precisely to the right or left of the goalie and fast, travelling at 90 to 104 kilometres per hour.
Anything faster than this boosts the chance of a miss because of inaccuracy, while anything slower helps the goalie to intercept it.
Moving swiftly to take the penalty (less than three seconds after the whistle is blown) gives the striker the element of surprise, while delaying the strike by more than 13 seconds makes the keeper unsettled, according to the researchers, who looked at decades of international matches involving England.
Waiting for the goalkeeper to move also boosted chances. However, waiting longer than 0.41 milliseconds caused a scoring chance to be halved. A run-up of four to six steps was the most successful approach, while a long run-up of 10 metres was the least.
Seen only through the prism of statistics, the balance in penalties is tilted massively in favour of the taker: between two-thirds and three-quarters of strikes result in a goal, according to various analyses in top-flight European club soccer.
But in a counter-intuitive way, these figures also give the psychological advantage to the keeper. If the penalty succeeds, people will pat him on the shoulder and say hard luck, because few expected him to save it. If he does save it, he will be praised to the rafters. In other words, all the onus lies with the penalty-taker.

Staying focused

This problem was explored last year by a team at the University of Exeter in south-west England, which asked members of the university football squad to wear special glasses, recording eye movements, while they took two series of penalties.
In the first series, the players were simply asked to do their best to score. In the second, they were told the results would be recorded and shared with the other players, with a bounty of 50 pounds for the best penalty-taker.
The more anxious the penalty-taker was, the likelier he was to look at and focus on the centrally-positioned goalkeeper. And because gaze control and motor control are tightly coordinated, the player's shot also centralised, making it far easier for the shot to be saved.
"The optimum strategy for penalty takers to use is to pick a spot and shoot to it, ignoring the goalkeeper in the process," says lead researcher Greg Wood.
Practice is essential, he says. The Hungarian great Ferenc Puskas would train again and again, shooting at a 25-centimetre disc hung 80 centimetres from the bar.
"The idea that you cannot recreate the anxiety a penalty-taker feels during a shootout is no excuse for not practising," Mr Wood says.
"Do you think other elite performers don't practice basic aiming shots in darts, snooker or golf for the same reasons? The skills need to be ingrained so they are robust under pressure."

Hips and colour

As for helping the goalie, experiments suggest looking at a players' hips during the end of the run-up gives a tip as to where the ball will be struck.
Researchers at the University of Hong Kong also suggest that if a keeper moves just six to 10 centimetres off centre, that is enough to tempt the taker into directing the kick to the side of the goal where there is more space.
There are also mind games, such as shuffling or taking time to prepare for the shot to distract the penalty-taker.
Even clothing colour is thought to be a help: Petr Cech of Chelsea prefers a bright orange strip in the belief that it attracts opponents and make them likelier to shoot straight at him.
That belief is bolstered by sports psychologists at the University of Chichester in southern England, who asked 40 footballers to take dozens of penalties over a week against a single keeper who changed strip. When the keeper wore red, only 54 per cent of the penalties scored; for yellow, it was 69 per cent, for blue 72 per cent and green 75 per cent.
Why? Red is associated with danger, dominance or anger, and at times of stress we pay more attention to it in our environment, goes the theory.